Mostrar el registro sencillo del ítem

dc.contributor.authorCallizo-Romero, Carmen
dc.contributor.authorTutnjević, Slavica
dc.contributor.authorPandza, Maja
dc.contributor.authorOuellet, Marc 
dc.contributor.authorKranjec, Alexander
dc.contributor.authorIlić, Sladjana
dc.contributor.authorGu, Yan
dc.contributor.authorGöksun, Tilbe
dc.contributor.authorChahboun, Sobh
dc.contributor.authorCasasanto, Daniel
dc.contributor.authorSantiago, Julio
dc.date.accessioned2025-01-17T08:43:52Z
dc.date.available2025-01-17T08:43:52Z
dc.date.issued2020-07-29
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/10481/99486
dc.descriptionOpen practices statement: The materials used for data collection, the raw data collected for the present study, analysis scripts for the results reported here, and supplementary tables and high-quality figures are available at https://osf.io/86mxd.es_ES
dc.description.abstractThe temporal focus hypothesis (TFH) proposes that whether the past or the future is conceptualized as being located in front depends on temporal focus: the balance of attention paid to the past (tradition) and the future (progress). How general is the TFH, and to what extent can cultures and subcultures be placed on a single line relating time spatialization and temporal focus in spite of stark differences in language, religion, history, and economic development? Data from 10Western (sub)cultural groups (N = 1198,) were used to derive a linear model relating aggregated temporal focus and proportion of future-in-front responses. This model then successfully fitted 10 independently collected (sub)cultural groups in China and Vietnam (N = 899). Further analysis of the whole data set (N = 2,097) showed that the group-level relation arose at the individual level and allowed precise quantification of its influence. Finally, in an effort to apply the model to all relevant published data sets, we included recent data from Britain and South Africa: The former, but not the latter, fitted the model well. Temporal focus is a central factor that shapes how people around the world think of time in spatial terms.es_ES
dc.description.sponsorshipThis research was supported by Grant No. PSI2015-67531-P from the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitivity to Julio Santiago (PI), Daniel Casasanto, Tilbe Göksun, Alexander Kranjec, Joseph Lavallee, Marc Ouellet, y Slavica Tutnjević, as well as by a predoctoral contract (BES-2016-076717) to Carmen Callizo-Romero.es_ES
dc.language.isoenges_ES
dc.publisherPsychonomic Bulletin & Reviewes_ES
dc.rightsAttribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 Internacional*
dc.rights.urihttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/*
dc.subjectCross-cultural differenceses_ES
dc.subjectTemporal focuses_ES
dc.subjectTime es_ES
dc.subjectSpacees_ES
dc.titleTemporal focus and time spatialization across cultureses_ES
dc.typejournal articlees_ES
dc.rights.accessRightsopen accesses_ES
dc.identifier.doi10.3758/s13423-020-01760-5


Ficheros en el ítem

[PDF]

Este ítem aparece en la(s) siguiente(s) colección(ones)

Mostrar el registro sencillo del ítem

Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 Internacional
Excepto si se señala otra cosa, la licencia del ítem se describe como Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 Internacional