Afficher la notice abrégée

dc.contributor.authorNarganes Pineda, Cristina
dc.contributor.authorChica Martínez, Ana Belén
dc.contributor.authorLupiáñez Castillo, Juan 
dc.contributor.authorMarotta, Andrea 
dc.date.accessioned2024-10-09T08:36:36Z
dc.date.available2024-10-09T08:36:36Z
dc.date.issued2022-02-22
dc.identifier.citationNarganes Pineda, C. et. al. Psychological Research 87, 242–259 (2023). [https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-022-01659-x]es_ES
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/10481/95731
dc.description.abstractArrows and gaze stimuli lead to opposite spatial congruency effects. While standard congruency effects are observed for arrows (faster responses for congruent conditions), responses are faster when eye-gaze stimuli are presented on the opposite side of the gazed-at location (incongruent trials), leading to a reversed congruency effect (RCE). Here, we explored the effects of implicit vs. explicit processing of arrows and eye-gaze direction. Participants were required to identify the direction (explicit task) or the colour (implicit task) of left or right looking/pointing gaze or arrows, presented to either the left or right of the fixation point. When participants responded to the direction of stimuli, standard congruency effects for arrows and RCE for eye-gaze stimuli were observed. However, when participants responded to the colour of stimuli, no congruency effects were observed. These results suggest that it is necessary to explicitly pay attention to the direction of eye-gaze and arrows for the congruency effect to occur. The same pattern of data was observed when participants responded either manually or verbally, demonstrating that manual motor components are not responsible for the results observed. These findings are not consistent with some hypotheses previously proposed to explain the RCE observed with eye-gaze stimuli and, therefore, call for an alternative plausible hypothesis.es_ES
dc.description.sponsorshipSpanish Ministry of Economy, Industry and Competitiveness, research projects PSI2017-88136-P to ABC and PSI2017-84926-P and PID2020-114790GB-I00es_ES
dc.description.sponsorshipAndalusian Council and European Regional Development Fund, through research project B-SEJ-572-UGR20es_ES
dc.description.sponsorshipFPU predoctoral fellowship from the Spanish Ministry of Education, Culture and Sport (FPU16/05056)es_ES
dc.language.isoenges_ES
dc.publisherSpringerLinkes_ES
dc.rightsAtribución 4.0 Internacional*
dc.rights.urihttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/*
dc.subjectArrowses_ES
dc.subjectGazees_ES
dc.subjectImplicit processinges_ES
dc.subjectAttentional orientinges_ES
dc.subjectSocial attentiones_ES
dc.titleExplicit vs. implicit spatial processing in arrow vs. eye‑gaze spatial congruency effectses_ES
dc.typejournal articlees_ES
dc.rights.accessRightsopen accesses_ES
dc.identifier.doi10.1007/s00426-022-01659-x
dc.type.hasVersionVoRes_ES


Fichier(s) constituant ce document

[PDF]

Ce document figure dans la(les) collection(s) suivante(s)

Afficher la notice abrégée

Atribución 4.0 Internacional
Excepté là où spécifié autrement, la license de ce document est décrite en tant que Atribución 4.0 Internacional